Duke University
Department of Psychology
Latin Honors Thesis
April 28, 1997
HTML Version: May 19, 1997
ABSTRACT
The purpose of this study was to determine whether mindfulness meditation training could reduce pain and distress in a laboratory finger pressure pain task, reduce severity of daily hassles, improve mood, and reduce psychological distress in a community sample. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: mindfulness meditation intervention or wait list. Participants in the mindfulness meditation intervention condition (n = 16) attended four weekly group sessions training them in the practice of mindfulness meditation. They also were asked to meditate 20 minutes daily. Wait list participants (n = 17) received no intervention. Measures of laboratory pain, daily hassles, mood, and psychological distress were collected from all participants before and after intervention. After two sets of measures had been collected, the wait list participants received the mindfulness meditation intervention. A third set of measures was then collected from the wait list participants. Results indicate that the participants in the intervention condition had decreased severity of daily hassles, improved mood, and decreased psychological distress at the second evaluation compared to the wait list participants. Results also indicate that participants in the wait list had greater decreases in distress for the second half of the laboratory pain task and greater improvements in mood during the intervention versus wait list phases of the study. Taken together, these findings indicate that mindfulness meditation training can reduce severity of daily hassles, improve mood, and reduce psychological distress and may be able to reduce distress during exposure to painful stimuli.
INTRODUCTION
Meditation can be defined as the
intentional self-regulation of attention from moment to moment (Kabat-Zinn,
1982). It is not, as is commonly believed, rumination or thinking about
a particular topic in a conceptual way, nor is it withdrawal of the senses
into a perceptually dormant state (this would be extinction of attention
rather than regulation). There are two main types of meditation: concentration
practices and mindfulness practices (Kabat-Zinn,
1982). As stated above, both involve the intentional regulation of
attention, but the manner in which attention is utilized differs fundamentally
between the two types of practice.
Concentration methods are among the
most widely known and researched forms of meditation. Transcendental Meditation
(TM) and the relaxation response (Benson &
Klipper, 1988) are examples of concentration practices. Concentration
practices are characterized by the focusing of attention onto a single
object such as a mantra (a mentally repeated phrase), a visual object,
or the experience of breathing. The object of concentration is held in
the mind throughout the meditation session, and any other type of mental
activity is viewed as a distraction to be guarded against.
Mindfulness meditation1
has its roots in Theravada Buddhism. Like concentration practices, mindfulness
also involves the concentration of attention, but it does not involve focusing
attention on one particular object. Instead, mindfulness involves maintaining
a moment-to-moment awareness of a constantly changing array of objects.
This is accomplished by first stabilizing attention on a primary object
(usually the breath) until the mind has settled somewhat and then gradually
expanding the field of attention to include all sensory and mental events.2
The attentional stance one takes toward these sensory
events is the crucial aspect of mindfulness meditation. The individuals
thoughts, physical sensations, emotions, memories, etc. are not to be regarded
in such a way as to promote interpretation or judgment of them. Rather
they are to be regarded with mindfulness, an attentional stance in which
sensations, feelings, and thoughts are observed as separate and distinct
from the expectations, desires, or aversions which arise in dependence
on them. Equanimity is cultivated as this choiceless awareness
(Krishnamurti, 1969) or bare attention
(Goldstein, 1987) that does not judge or
categorize perception but rather observes experience as being in moment-to-moment
flux. All thoughts and experiences which arise are treated the same; no
relative or absolute value is placed upon any of them. This specialized
use of attention is the essence of mindfulness meditation. (For a more
thorough explanation of mindfulness meditation, refer to Kabat-Zinn
(1982).)
From this description, it should be
evident that mindfulness presupposes proficiency in concentration practices.
In this regard, it coincides with the central features of TM and the relaxation
response, namely a mental device and a passive attitude (Benson
& Klipper, 1988). However, mindfulness differs from the concentration
practices in that it does not have a constant object of attention. In mindfulness,
there is no distraction to be guarded against because any mental event
can be taken as the object of mindfulness.
Pain and Meditation
There is physiological evidence
supporting the theory that the experience of pain is a result of three
interacting dimensions: sensory-discriminative, affective-motivational, and
cognitive-interpretative (Melzack & Wall,
1982). According to the gate control theory of pain, pain signals must
pass through a gate in the spinal cord that can be closed partially
or completely, thus blocking the transmission of pain sensations to the
brain (Melzack & Wall, 1982). Activities
in the affective and cognitive domains (e.g. distraction, hypnosis, or
relaxation) can send descending signals from the brain to the spinal cord
which close the gate and affect the sensory dimension of the pain experience.
Intensive practitioners of meditation
may experience pain caused by sitting motionless for long periods of time,
often in a cross-legged position. Traditional meditation texts contain
instructions for cultivating equanimity toward this sometimes intense pain
through the specialized regulation of attention which characterizes mindfulness
meditation (Nyanaponika, 1962). Kabat-Zinn
(1982) suggests that insights gained from the observation of pain arising
during meditation might serve as a model for a strategy that patients may
use for coping with chronic pain. The present author suggests that people
not suffering from chronic pain might also use these meditative insights
to develop a strategy for coping with acute pain which they may experience.
Mindfulness meditation requires focusing
on unpleasant and painful sensations when they are present and discourages
attempts to escape them by distraction or by focusing on some other object
of attention (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). The success
of this technique as a pain coping strategy would depend upon the ability
of the person to maintain a stance of bare attention toward the sensations,
that is, to be able to separate the sensory dimension of the pain experience
from the other dimensions and to view the physical sensation with equanimity.
With practice, the person could develop the ability to assume this attentional
stance toward physical sensation when it became dominant in the field of
awareness and to similarly observe the accompanying but independent cognitive
processes which lead to the evaluation and labeling of the sensation as
painful. According to this understanding of mindfulness meditation, one
would expect the nociceptive signals to remain undiminished, but the emotional
and cognitive components of the pain experience to be reduced.
Kabat-Zinn developed a Stress Reduction
and Relaxation Program (SR&RP)3 which employs a
10-week training program in mindfulness meditation for patients who are
referred by their physicians (Kabat-Zinn, 1982).
Chronic pain is one of the primary reasons for referral to the program.
Patient outcomes for the SR&RP have provided tentative support for
the use of mindfulness meditation as a therapy for chronic pain (Kabat-Zinn,
1982; Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, & Burney,
1985; Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, Burney, &
Sellers, 1986). However, this research did not employ random group
assignments. In addition, the SR&RP also included training in Hatha
Yoga, which may effectively reduce pain by itself (Garfinkel,
Schumacher, Husain, Levy, & Reshetar, 1994).
Leventhal
and Everhart (1980) have proposed a parallel processing
model of pain which is useful in understanding how mindfulness meditation
might elicit changes in pain perception. According to this model, sensations
produced by painful stimuli can be processed in two ways: as objective
sensations or as distressing, painful sensations. To the extent that persons
can conceptualize the sensations in an objective rather than a distressing
way, the emotional upset that usually accompanies the experience of those
sensations is reduced. Laboratory studies using the cold pressor stimulus
have shown that a strategy of attention to primary sensation during exposure
to painful stimulus results in significantly less distress and higher tolerance
than strategies of distraction or emotional expression (Ahles,
Blanchard, & Leventhal, 1983). Participants assigned to the attention
group in this study were not given any training but were simply told to
describe aloud the sensations that they were experiencing. Pain ratings
did not differ significantly between groups, but the distress ratings dropped
significantly for the attention group in relation to the other groups beginning
at around two minutes. McCaul and Haugtvedt (1982)
produced analogous decreases in distress beginning after two minutes of
stimulus exposure and also found that attention to sensation becomes a
relatively better coping strategy as the trial becomes longer.
It has been suggested that mindfulness
meditation represents a more robust strategy of observing sensations than
that used in the above studies and that, as a result, it might produce
more dramatic decreases in distress (Kabat-Zinn,
et al., 1985). A previous study of concentration meditation comparing
the responses of practitioners of TM with non-practitioners during acute
cold pressor pain revealed that meditators reported significantly lower
levels of distress but not pain (Mills & Farrow,
1981). However, the present study is the first randomized controlled
study to investigate the effects of mindfulness meditation upon laboratory
trials involving acute pain stimulation.
Daily Hassles and Meditation
Previous research has established
the ability of concentration meditation to decrease stress via the relaxation
response (Benson, 1983; Delmonte,
1984) and has suggested that mindfulness meditation can reduce stress-related
symptoms such as tension headache (Sharma, Kumaraiah,
Mishra, & Balodhi, 1990). However, no research has been conducted
examining the effects of mindfulness meditation on perceptions of daily
hassles. Daily hassles are defined as the relatively minor stresses that
characterize everyday life (Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer,
and Lazarus, 1981). These day-to-day events may be a good global indicator
of stress and have been found to be better predictors of health outcomes
than other forms of stress measurement (Kanner,
et al., 1981). There are now standard ways of measuring daily hassles,
such as the Hassles Scale (Kanner, et al., 1981).
Because all people experience stress
arising from daily hassles, it would be of great potential benefit to know
if mindfulness meditation was able to attenuate the perception of daily
hassles. The specialized use of attention that characterizes mindfulness
meditation should result in changes in the cognitive appraisal of daily
hassles because in mindfulness meditation, much as in cognitive therapy,
external events are observed to be separate from the cognitive and affective
processes which they produce. This separation of the components of the
experience of daily hassles should lead to decreases in the perception
of their severity. Because no controlled studies have been conducted in
this area, the present study examined how training in mindfulness meditation
affected the perceived severity of daily hassles.
Mood and Meditation
The effects of meditation on mood have been studied extensively. Mindfulness meditation has been found to result in decreased mood disturbance and depression and increased self-esteem in chronic pain patients (Kabat-Zinn, et al., 1985). Meditation which elicits the relaxation response has also been found to result in reductions in hostility (Barr & Benson, 1984). Again, however, there is an absence of randomized controlled studies examining these effects in community samples. It seems reasonable, however, that the cognitive restructuring involved in mindfulness meditation would elicit favorable changes in mood in normal individuals as well. The possible presence of these effects was examined in present study.
Psychological Distress and Meditation
Mindfulness meditation has been repeatedly found to be helpful in reducing psychological distress. Several authors have reported that meditation can result in decreases in anxiety, panic, and psychological symptomatology in individuals suffering from anxiety disorders or depression (Kabat-Zinn, Massion, Kristeller, Peterson, Fletcher, Pbert, Lenderking, & Santorelli, 1992; Kutz, 1985). No studies have been conducted in community samples, but it seems reasonable that similar results would be found in normal individuals practicing mindfulness meditation. Therefore, the present study examined changes in psychological distress accompanying training in mindfulness meditation.
METHODS
Because of the lack of randomized controlled research on mindfulness meditation in community samples, it seemed worthwhile to investigate the effects mindfulness meditation might have on pain perception, daily hassles, mood, and psychological distress. In the study presented here, participants attended a four-week mindfulness meditation training program. Before and after the training, they were exposed to a laboratory finger pressure pain task in order to see how meditation training affected their ratings of pain and distress. It was anticipated that ratings of distress (but not ratings of pain) during the finger pressure task following meditation training would decrease after two minutes of stimulus exposure. Participants also filled out questionnaires before and after the training in order to examine effects on daily hassles, mood, and psychological distress. Decreases in severity of daily hassles were expected along with overall improvements in mood and psychological distress.
Participants
Thirty-three community volunteers
(21 female and 12 male) were recruited through fliers and newspaper advertisements.
Participants were not compensated for study participation. This research
was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the American
Psychological Association for research involving human participants.
The average age of participants was
29.36 years (SD = 11.69). The ethnic distribution was 76% Caucasian,
9% African American, 9% Asian American, and 6% other. The highest level
of education of participants was as follows: 21% held a masters degree,
33% held a bachelors degree, 6% held an associates degree, 33% were currently
enrolled undergraduates, and 6% held a high school diploma. No participants
had a regular meditation practice regimen prior to entering the study.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups: meditation intervention
(n = 16) or wait list control (n = 17). Because it was believed
that pain and distress ratings might be affected by gender (Dubreuil
& Kohn, 1986) and handedness (Haslam, 1970),
participants were grouped by these variables before being randomized.
Intervention
The mindfulness meditation training
program consisted of four weekly group sessions each lasting for two hours.
The content of sessions was based partly on that of the SR&RP. Participants
received instruction in the method and practice of mindfulness meditation,
were presented with didactic material relating meditation to physical and
psychological well-being, engaged in walking and sitting meditation, and
participated in open question and answer sessions intended to help troubleshoot
difficulties related to their practice. A non-goal orientation was emphasized
as being the best approach for practicing mindfulness and the best way
to benefit from it. Each session focused on a different aspect of mindfulness
practice.
The first session explained the relaxation
response and the potential health benefits to be gained from meditation
practice. Mindfulness meditation was introduced as a powerful vehicle for
self-inquiry and self-understanding based on moment-to-moment awareness.
Breath awareness meditation was taught as a method for concentrating and
stabilizing the mind, and participants were guided through a 20-minute
sitting meditation. The session ended with an open question and answer
period. Participants were then given an audio tape containing a 20-minute
guided meditation and were asked to meditate for 20 minutes a day either
with or without the tape. Participants were also given meditation diaries
to keep track of their practice and to record anything that they wanted
to discuss during the group sessions.
The second session focused on how mindfulness
can be applied to physical sensations and to emotions. The session began
with an open question and answer period in which participants received
help troubleshooting difficulties or addressing questions they had about
the practice. After this, an explanation of the gate control theory of
pain perception was given, followed by a discussion of the malleability
of physical and emotional experience. Mindfulness of sensations was introduced
as a method of developing a non-judging awareness of ones experiences.
Participants were guided in a 20-minute sitting meditation which consisted
of breath awareness and awareness of sensations and feelings. This was
followed by a 10-minute walking meditation to further practice awareness
of sensations. The session concluded with another open question and answer
period.
The third session focused on the application
of mindfulness to ones thoughts. Following an initial question and answer
period, there was a presentation on the role thoughts play in shaping our
experiences and on how differences in the cognitive labeling of a stimulus
can lead to very different experiences of that stimulus. A noting
method of becoming aware of the presence of thoughts without becoming caught
up in their content was taught as a way of cultivating mindfulness towards
thoughts. Participants were led in a 20-minute sitting and 10-minute walking
meditation that reinforced all of the mindfulness techniques that had been
taught so far. An open question and answer session followed.
During the fourth session, the topic
of integrating mindfulness into everyday life was addressed. The session
began with an open question and answer period. This was followed by a discussion
of changes participants had noticed in their daily activities since beginning
to practice mindfulness meditation. There was then a presentation on how
mindfulness could be cultivated during ordinary daily activities in order
to bring a greater fullness to the meditation practice. Participants were
asked to share stories related to the practice of mindfulness during ordinary
activities and how this affected how they felt during the activity. The
session ended with a question and answer session, and participants were
given a list of readings and resources related to mindfulness meditation.
Procedure
Upon arriving at the laboratory, participants were informed that the purpose of the study was to examine the effects of mindfulness meditation on pain perception, daily hassles, mood, and psychological distress. Participants read and signed consent forms explaining the purpose, method, and requirements of the study, including that the study would involve exposure to a laboratory finger pressure pain task. After consenting, participants participated in an evaluation session in which they completed a biographical information form, the Hassles Scale, the POMS, the SCL-90-R, and the laboratory finger pressure pain task. Participants in the intervention group entered the four week mindfulness meditation training program. Participants in the wait list group received no intervention during the initial four week period. After the initial four weeks had elapsed, all participants returned to the laboratory for a second evaluation session in which the same measures used in the initial session were collected. Following the second evaluation, participants in the wait list group received the four week mindfulness meditation training program and then returned to the laboratory for a third evaluation session.
Evaluation Measures
Hassles Scale
The Hassles Scale (Kanner,
et al., 1981) is a 117-item self-report questionnaire designed to measure
the impact of minor hassles of everyday life in the areas of work, family,
social activities, finances, etc. (e.g. concerns about owing money,
having to wait, and job dissatisfactions). Participants
were asked to circle each hassle they had experienced in the past month,
leaving blank all items that were not applicable to them. For each item
circled, participants rated the severity of the hassle on a three-point
scale (1 = somewhat severe, 2 = moderately severe,
3 = extremely severe). The numeric values of the responses
were summed to derive a total hassles score, the Cumulated Severity Index
(CSI). Test-retest reliability and construct validity of the Hassles Scale
have been supported in previous research (DeLongis,
Coyne, Dakof, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1982; Kanner
et al., 1981).
Profile of Mood States
The Profile of Mood States (POMS;
McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1971) is a
self-report questionnaire that measures mood on several subscales. The
POMS Short Form used in this study consists of a list of 30 words describing
feelings people have. Participants were asked to rate each item on a five-point
scale (0 = not at all to 4 = extremely) according
to how they had been feeling during the past week. The numerical values
of responses are summed to produce scores on the following six subscales:
tension-anxiety, anger-hostility, fatigue-inertia, vigor-activity, confusion-bewilderment,
and depression-dejection. A Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) score that is
a good global estimate of affective state is obtainable by summing the
subscale scores (McNair et al., 1971). The
POMS has been used extensively to measure mood change in applied clinical
research, and its internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and validity
have been supported in prior research (Malouff,
Schuttle, & Ramerth, 1985; McNair et al.,
1971).
Symptom Checklist
The Symptom Checklist-90-Revised
(SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1983) is a 90-item
self-report inventory designed to assess a wide variety of psychological
symptoms (e.g. feeling that people are unfriendly or dislike you,
sleep that is restless or disturbed, and spells of terror
or panic). Participants were asked to rate how distressed they had
been by each symptom during the past week on a five-point scale (0 = not
at all to 4 = extremely). The Global Severity Index (GSI)
is the mean rating calculated across all 90 items and is the best single
indicator of the current level or depth of disorder (Derogatis,
1983). In addition, nine subscales can be derived: somatization, obsessive-compulsive,
interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety,
paranoid ideation, and psychoticism. Internal consistency, test-retest
reliability, and concurrent validity for the SCL-90-R have been found to
be good (Derogatis, 1983).
Laboratory Finger Pressure Pain Task
A Forgione-Barber strain gauge
pain stimulator (see Figure 1) was used in the
laboratory finger pressure pain task (Forgione
& Barber, 1971). The device consists of a dull Lucite knife edge
(.25 mm thick) attached to an adjustable lever arm. The knife edge exerts
a constant pressure on the second phalanx of any finger, resulting in a
sensation of slight pressure that gradually builds into a dull, aching
pain. The rate at which the sensation becomes painful can be varied by
changing the magnitude of weight attached to the lever arm. Investigators
have found the Forgione-Barber stimulator to be reliable, to produce sensations
that more closely approximate clinical pain, and to be relatively unaffected
by vasomotor activity and other extraneous physiological events (Forgione
& Barber, 1971; Merskey, 1974). In
experimental trials, two weights (40 g and 70 g) were used for pain stimulation.
These weights have been used by previous investigators (Malow,
West, & Sutker, 1987; Malow, West, &
Sutker, 1989). Both weights were externally identical so that participants
could not distinguish them visually.4 All trials were
held between the hours of five oclock and eight oclock in the evening
in order to avoid possible time of day effects on pain perception (Labrecque
& Vanier, 1995).
Figure 1. The Forgione-Barber strain gauge pain stimulator.
Participants were led into another
room for the laboratory finger pressure pain task. The room contained a
round table with one chair facing it. On the table lay the Forgione-Barber
strain gauge pain stimulator as well as two 11-point visual analog scales,
one labeled pain and the other labeled distress. The endpoints of the pain
scale were labeled with verbal descriptors (0 = No pain and
10 = Pain as bad as it can be) as were those of the distress
scale (0 = No distress and 10 = Maximum you can imagine).
After being seated, participants were read instructions explaining the
trial apparatus and procedure. They were assured that there was no risk
of any harm to their finger and that they could stop the trial at any time
by saying, Stop. Participants were told that their finger would
be placed inside the device for four minutes on two separate trials and
that there would be a four minute rest between trials. They were instructed
to make pain and distress ratings using the visual scales each time the
experimenter announced, Judgment. Pain was described as the
physical intensity of what you will be feeling, while distress was
the amount of mental distress or upset that the sensations are causing
you. Participants were told that each rating was independent of the
others and that their ratings could go up, down, or remain the same. These
instructions were adapted from Leventhal, Brown,
Shacham, & Enquist (1979). At no testing session were participants
instructed to use mindfulness techniques during the laboratory finger pressure
pain task.
After the instructions were read, the
middle finger of the participants left hand was inserted into the pain
stimulator. The left hands of all participants were used because of prior
findings that the left hand is more sensitive to pain regardless of handedness
(Murray & Safferstone, 1970). The experimenter
placed the 40 g weight on the lever arm and sat approximately 6 feet to
the side and rear of the participants so that they would not be distracted
by the experimenters presence. The experimenter announced, Judgment,
at twenty second intervals and recorded participants verbal ratings of
pain and distress. After a four minute rest, the procedure was repeated
with the 70 g weight. No participants requested the trial to stop before
the maximum four minutes had elapsed for either weight. Two summary scores
were calculated for the pain ratings and for the distress ratings: (a)
an overall mean which was the mean of ratings given for both weights for
the entire trial, and (b) a mean for the last half of the trial which was
the mean of ratings given for both weights beginning two minutes into the
trial.
Immediately following the laboratory
finger pressure pain task, participants completed a coping strategies checklist
that was created to assess whether they had employed distraction, sensory
attention, or relaxation techniques during stimulus exposure. The checklist
contained three items: (a) While my finger was in the device, I tried
to find other things to look at or think about and to ignore what was going
on with my finger; (b) While my finger was in the device, I
tried to concentrate on all the sensations that it was experiencing--sensations
such as stinging, burning, throbbing, etc.; and (c) While my
finger was in the device, I tried to relax myself in order to keep my pain
and distress from increasing. Participants were instructed to mark
each strategy that they had used. It was anticipated that participants
employing mindfulness meditation techniques would mark the sensory attention
item and perhaps the relaxation item but that they would not mark the distraction
item.
RESULTS
There was no participant loss between
the first and second evaluations. However, six participants assigned to
the wait list group dropped out of the study between the second and third
evaluation sessions when they were scheduled to receive the mindfulness
meditation intervention. All of these participants reported having time
conflicts with the group sessions. However, the data gathered for these wait list
participants during the first two evaluation sessions were retained in analyses.
Data analysis was conducted in three
steps. The first step evaluated pre-intervention differences between the
groups. The second step compared outcomes at evaluation 2 for participants
in the intervention and wait list groups. The third step examined data
collected from participants in the wait list condition to determine if
they showed significant changes in outcome during the intervention versus
wait list phases of the study.
Analysis of Initial Differences Between Groups
Table 1 shows the initial group means and standard deviations for age, gender, handedness, pain and distress ratings, Hassles Scale CSI scores, POMS TMD scores, and SCL-90-R GSI scores. T-test comparisons (or chi-square analyses, as appropriate) were performed on these variables and on all subscales of the POMS and SCL-90-R. There were no significant initial differences between groups on any of these variables.
Table 1
Pre-intervention means and standard deviations for both groups for pain
and distress ratings and Hassles Scale, POMS, and SCL-90-R scores
Notice that the initial levels of distress were very low for both groups. On a scale of 0 to 10, the intervention group reported an overall mean distress level of 1.9 (SD = 1.2) for the finger pressure pain task and a mean of 2.4 (SD = 1.3) for the last half of the finger pressure pain task. Similarly, the wait list group reported an overall mean distress level of 2.4 (SD = 2.1) for the finger pressure pain task and a mean of 2.8 (SD = 2.5) for the last half of the finger pressure pain task.
Post-Intervention (Evaluation 2) Differences Between Groups
Effects of Intervention on Pain and Distress During the Laboratory
Finger Pressure Pain Task
Effects of intervention on distress
ratings. It was hypothesized that the intervention would result in
decreased distress ratings during the last half of the finger pressure
pain task. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted to examine
post-intervention between group differences in overall mean distress ratings
during the finger pressure pain task. In this analysis, participants pre-intervention
(i.e. evaluation 1) distress ratings were used as a covariate. The results
of the ANCOVA revealed that the between group effect was not significant
(F(1, 30) = .818, p < .38). Figure
2A displays the adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) means for
the intervention and wait list groups. As can be seen, participants in
the two groups had similar overall mean distress ratings.
Figure 2A. Adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean distress ratings during the finger pressure pain task.
To examine if differences in distress
ratings were evident for the last half of the finger pressure pain task,
a second ANCOVA was conducted examining between group differences in mean
distress ratings for the last half of the laboratory finger pressure pain
task. The results of this ANCOVA revealed that the between group effect
was not significant (F(1, 30) = 1.418, p < .25). Figure
2A displays the adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) means for
the intervention and wait list groups. As can be seen, the differences
between groups in distress ratings for the last half of the finger pressure
pain task, although not statistically significant, were in the expected
direction and were somewhat larger than the differences between groups
for the overall mean distress ratings. To determine if the results differed
as a function of the weights beings judged, separate ANCOVAs were performed
on distress rating data collected for the 40 g and 70 g weights individually.
No significant between group effects were obtained.
Effects of intervention on pain
ratings. It was hypothesized that the intervention would not have an
effect on pain ratings. An ANCOVA was conducted to examine post-intervention
between group differences in overall mean pain ratings during the finger
pressure pain task. The results of the ANCOVA revealed that the between
group effect was not significant (F(1, 30) = .909, p <
.35). Figure 2B displays the adjusted post-intervention
(evaluation 2) mean pain ratings for the intervention and wait list groups.
As can be seen, participants in the two groups had similar overall mean
pain ratings.
Figure 2B. Adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean pain ratings during the finger pressure pain task.
To examine if differences in pain
ratings were evident for the last half of the finger pressure pain task,
a second ANCOVA was conducted examining between group differences in mean
pain ratings for the last half of the laboratory finger pressure pain task.
The results of this ANCOVA revealed that the between group effect was not
significant (F(1, 30) = .949, p < .34). Figure
2B displays the adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) means for
the intervention and wait list groups. As can be seen, participants in
the two groups had similar pain ratings for the last half of the trial.
To determine if the results differed
as a function of the weights beings judged, separate ANCOVAs were performed
on pain rating data collected for the 40 g and 70 g weights individually.
No significant between group effects were obtained.
Coping Strategies Checklist
Participants were expected to use
more sensory attention strategies and less distraction during stimulus
exposure following intervention. Table 2 shows the
number of participants marking each item of the coping strategies checklist
at each evaluation. As can be seen in the first two columns, the decrease
in distraction and increase in sensory attention use for the intervention
group was minimal. Chi-square comparisons of the distraction (p
< .35), sensory attention (p < .35), and relaxation (p
< .15) items were not significant. This suggests that the intervention
may not have been effective in increasing participants use of mindfulness
techniques during the laboratory finger pressure pain task. As expected,
the wait list group showed no significant changes between evaluations 1
and 2 on the distraction (p < .27)), sensory attention (p
< .72), or relaxation (p < .36) items.
Table 2
Number of participants responding to each item of the coping strategies
checklist5
Intervention Compliance
Participants in the intervention
group performed the mindfulness meditation practice an average of 4.3 days
(SD = 2.1) per week out of the recommended 7 days.
Effects of Intervention on Daily Hassles, Mood, and Psychological
Distress
Effects of intervention on daily
hassles. It was hypothesized that meditation training would result
in decreased scores on the daily hassles measure. An ANCOVA conducted to
analyze the Hassles Scale CSI scores revealed a significant between group
effect (F(1, 30) = 11.13, p < .003). Figure
3 displays the adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean CSI scores.
Participants in the intervention group rated the severity of their daily
hassles as significantly lower than participants in the wait list group.
Figure 3. Adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean Hassles Scale CSI scores.
Effects of intervention on mood. It was hypothesized that meditation training would result in decreased scores on the mood measure. An ANCOVA conducted to analyze the POMS TMD scores revealed a significant between group effect (F(1, 30) = 9.57, p < .004). Figure 4 displays the adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean TMD scores. Participants in the intervention group reported significantly lower levels of mood disturbance than participants in the wait list group.
Figure 4. Adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean POMS TMD scores.
To examine if differences in mood
were evident on the subscales of the POMS, a multivariate analysis of covariance
(MANCOVA) was conducted. In this analysis, participants pre-intervention
(i.e. evaluation 1) scores on the POMS subscales were used as covariates.
The overall between group effect for post-intervention (i.e. evaluation
2) POMS subscales was not significant (Wilks lambda F(6, 20) =
1.78, p < .16).
Effects of intervention on psychological
distress. It was hypothesized that meditation training would result
in decreased scores on the psychological distress measure. An ANCOVA conducted
to analyze the SCL-90-R GSI scores revealed a significant between group
effect (F(1, 30) = 8.87, p < .006). Figure
5 displays the adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean GSI scores.
Participants in the intervention group rated the severity of their psychological
distress as significantly lower than participants in the wait list group.
Figure 5. Adjusted post-intervention (evaluation 2) mean SCL-90-R GSI scores.
To examine if differences in psychological distress were evident on the subscales of the SCL-90-R, a MANCOVA was conducted. The overall between group effect for post-intervention (i.e. evaluation 2) SCL-90-R subscales was not significant (Wilks lambda F(9, 14) = 1.19, p < .38).
Analysis of Intervention Outcome in Wait List Group: Comparison of Changes in Outcome During Wait List Versus Intervention Phases of the Study
To examine whether improvements
in outcome were obtained in the wait list participants after they participated
in the intervention phase of the study, a series of analyses were carried
out. Change scores were computed for each outcome measure for the two phases
of the study: (a) change over the wait list phase (computed by subtracting
evaluation 2 data from evaluation 1 data), and (b) change over the intervention
phase (computed by subtracting evaluation 3 data from evaluation 2 data).
A series of one-sided t-tests was then conducted to compare the change
scores obtained during the wait list and intervention phases of the study.
Data from six participants who dropped out of the study between evaluations
2 and 3 were excluded in these analyses. In order to make sure that the
participants who dropped out did not differ from the rest of the wait list
group, t-test comparisons (or chi-square analyses, as appropriate) were
performed comparing the dropouts with the rest of the wait list participants.
Age, gender, handedness, initial (i.e. evaluation 1) values for overall
pain and distress ratings, initial values for pain and distress ratings
for the last half of trials, and initial CSI, TMD, and GSI scores were
examined. No significant differences were found between the dropouts and
the rest of the wait list participants on any of these variables.
Table 3 shows
the means and standard deviations for the wait list participants for changes
in pain and distress ratings, Hassles Scale CSI scores, POMS TMD scores,
and SCL-90-R GSI scores during the wait list and intervention phases of
the study.
Table 3
Means and standard deviations for wait list participants for changes
in scores during the wait list and intervention phases of the study
Effects of Intervention on Pain and Distress During the Laboratory
Finger Pressure Pain Task
Effects of intervention on distress
ratings. A t-test comparing changes in overall mean distress ratings
during the finger pressure pain task approached significance (t(1, 10)
= 1.74, p < .057). Figure 6A presents
the mean changes for both phases of the study. As can be seen, the means
differed somewhat, with the wait list group showing greater decreases in
overall distress ratings for the finger pressure pain task following the
intervention versus wait list phases of the study.
Figure 6A. Changes in distress ratings for wait list participants during wait list and intervention phases of the study.
A t-test comparing changes in distress
ratings for the last half of the finger pressure pain task was found to
be significant (t(1, 10) = 1.85, p < .047). Figure
6A presents the mean changes for both phases of the study. As can be
seen, the means differed markedly, with the wait list group showing greater
decreases in distress for the last half of the finger pressure pain task
following the intervention versus wait list phases of the study.
Effects of intervention on pain
ratings. A t-test comparing changes in overall mean pain ratings during
the finger pressure pain task was not significant (t(1, 10) = -.05, p
< .53). Figure 6B presents the mean changes
for both phases of the study. As can be seen, the means were very similar.
Figure 6B. Changes in pain ratings for wait list participants during wait list and intervention phases of the study.
A t-test comparing changes in pain ratings for the last half of the finger pressure pain task was also not significant (t(1, 10) = .25, p < .41). Figure 6B presents the mean changes for both phases of the study. As can be seen, the mean decrease for the intervention phase was slightly larger than that for the wait list phase.
Coping Strategies Checklist
Table 4
shows the changes in the number of wait list participants marking each
item of the coping strategies checklist. As can be seen, there was an increase
in the number of participants using sensory attention, and a slight decrease
in the use of distraction and relaxation strategies. Chi-square analyses
could not be conducted on these changes because of the low cell counts.
Table 4
Changes in the number of wait list participants marking each item of
the coping strategies checklist
Intervention Compliance
Participants in the wait list group
performed the mindfulness meditation practice an average of 4.6 days (SD
= .9) per week out of the recommended 7 days.
Effects of Intervention on Daily Hassles, Mood, and Psychological
Distress
Effects of intervention on daily
hassles. A t-test comparing changes in Hassles Scale CSI scores approached
significance (t(1, 10) = 1.72, p < .058). Figure
7 presents the mean changes for both phases of the study. As can be
seen, the means differed somewhat, with the wait list group showing greater
decreases in CSI scores following the intervention versus wait list phases
of the study.
Figure 7. Changes in Hassles Scale CSI scores for wait list participants during wait list and intervention phases of the study.
Effects of intervention on mood. A t-test comparing changes in POMS TMD scores was significant (t(1, 10) = 2.69, p < .012). Figure 8 presents the mean changes for both phases of the study. As can be seen, the means differed markedly, with the wait list group showing greater decreases in TMD scores following the intervention versus wait list phases of the study.
Figure 8. Changes in POMS TMD scores for wait list participants during wait list and intervention phases of the study.
Effects of intervention on psychological distress. A t-test comparing changes in SCL-90-R GSI scores was not significant (t(1, 10) = 1.36, p < .11). Figure 9 presents the mean changes for both phases of the study. As can be seen, the means differed only slightly, but the differences were in the expected direction, with the wait list group showing greater decreases in GSI scores following the intervention versus wait list phases of the study.
Figure 9. Changes in SCL-90-R GSI scores for wait list participants during wait list and intervention phases of the study.
DISCUSSION
The results of this study indicate
that the intervention group showed decreased severity of daily hassles,
improved mood, and decreased psychological distress following training
in mindfulness meditation. During the intervention phase of the study,
the wait list group also showed significant improvements in mood compared
to the wait list phase. This group also showed decreased distress ratings
during the last half of the finger pressure pain task following the intervention
phase and nearly significant decreases in severity of daily hassles.
The improvements in severity of daily
hassles, mood, and psychological distress following meditation training
are notable for two reasons. First, the intervention was not very intensive:
a four-week mindfulness meditation training program consisting of weekly
group sessions. In contrast, the SR&RP, which has served as a model
for previous investigators, consists of a 10-week training program. Second,
compliance with the daily mindfulness meditation practice was only moderate.
On average, participants practiced only about four out of the seven recommended
days per week. Nonetheless, participants exhibited substantial improvements
in severity of daily hassles, mood, and psychological distress, suggesting
that even a brief course of mindfulness meditation may be effective in
modifying these outcomes. The outcomes obtained would probably be even
more impressive if a more intensive intervention was used and if compliance
with the meditation practice could be increased.
The findings regarding mindfulness
meditation trainings effectiveness in decreasing distress caused by painful
stimuli were mixed. No significant effect was observed when comparing the
intervention groups distress ratings to the wait list groups. However,
after the wait list group was given the four-week intervention, they showed
significant decreases in distress during the last half of the finger pressure
pain task. They also showed nearly significant decreases in overall distress
ratings during the finger pressure pain task following intervention. However,
the results obtained from the wait list group must be interpreted cautiously
because there were six dropouts from this group during the intervention
phase. It is possible that the dropouts self-selected for some unknown
reason and that those participants would not have exhibited comparable
decreases in distress ratings. In addition, it is not possible to rule
out that the observed decreases in distress ratings for the wait list group
were due to the fact that they were exposed to the finger pressure pain
task three times rather than two as in the intervention group. Perhaps
with greater familiarity, the task was less distressing.
There were several methodological issues
encountered during the present study which may have contributed to the
failure to find the expected decreases in distress during the finger pressure
pain task for the intervention group. First, the weights used for the finger
pressure pain task elicited unexpectedly low overall distress ratings.
This appears to have created a floor effect, making it difficult to detect
possible trends in the distress ratings. In future research, heavier weights
should be used in order to elicit higher distress ratings and prevent such
floor effects.
Second, participants were not instructed
to use the mindfulness meditation techniques during the finger pressure
pain task. As the results of the coping strategies checklist show, participants
did not increase their use of the sensory attention techniques which were
taught in the mindfulness meditation intervention during the post-intervention
finger pressure pain task. This made it difficult to determine if the lack
of decrease in distress ratings was due to a problem with the mindfulness
techniques themselves or due to the participants simply failing to use
the techniques they had been taught. In future research, it would be helpful
to examine the effect of instructing participants to use the mindfulness
meditation intervention techniques during the finger pressure pain task.
It would also be helpful to include more material during the intervention
training specifically addressing the use of mindfulness as a coping strategy
for acute pain.
Third, participants in the study were
only asked to meditate for 20 minutes daily. In the SR&RP, patients
are asked to meditate for a minimum of 45 minutes per day. Since it was
hypothesized that mindfulnesss success as a pain coping strategy would
depend upon the ability of participants to maintain a stance of bare attention
toward the sensations, it is possible that participants in the present
study were not asked to practice enough during the intervention to develop
the attentional control required to effectively use mindfulness as a pain
coping strategy.
Fourth, the visual analog scale used
to measure distress during the finger pressure pain task may not have been
effective in separating the sensory and affective components of the pain
experience. Previous research has suggested that visual analog scales may
be less able to discriminate between pain intensity and pain affect than
other types of pain measures such as verbal rating scales (Duncan,
Bushnell, & Lavigne, 1989). Because the distress scale used in
the present study consisted of only one item, it may have failed to detect
a wide spectrum of other possible affective responses to the finger pressure
pain task. A multiple item pain measure such as the adjective checklist
of the McGill Pain Questionnaire might have been more sensitive in measuring
the affective component of participants pain perception.
The present study was also limited
by the reliance on a laboratory pain task instead of real-life pain. The
results obtained would be more generalizable if a study was conducted examining
how training in mindfulness meditation affects coping during exposure to
pain encountered in the course of ordinary life. In addition, the current
study focused solely on acute pain. Controlled research into the effects
of mindfulness meditation on chronic pain coping would be helpful in gaining
a clearer understanding of meditations mechanism of action and of its
potential applications as a pain coping strategy.
Although the present study yielded
only mixed results regarding mindfulness meditations effectiveness in
reducing distress caused by painful stimuli, the problems encountered in
this pilot study should be useful for investigators conducting future research
into the effects of mindfulness meditation on pain perception. Further,
even if it were shown that, in fact, mindfulness meditation had no effect
on pain perception, the results of the present study would still be potentially
important to both normal individuals and pain patients. Even with a small
number of participants, the present study demonstrated that a brief mindfulness
meditation training program can produce significant improvements in perceptions
of daily hassles, mood, and psychological distress. Because all people
are subject to daily hassles, negative moods, and psychological distress,
mindfulness meditation has widespread potential applications. This is especially
true of chronic pain patients, for whom physicians are often unable to
treat the underlying cause of their pain effectively and must instead attempt
to lessen their patients suffering by improving their quality of life.
Mindfulness meditation could be a useful self-management tool in such cases
to help reduce patients psychological suffering.
FOOTNOTES
1 Mindfulness
meditation, insight meditation, and awareness meditation are alternate
translations of the same Sanskrit term, vipassana.
2 In Buddhist
psychology there are six senses (sight, smell, touch, taste, hearing, and
mind), the objects of which together comprise all of an individuals
experience.
3 Division of Preventive
and Behavioral Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Massachusetts
Medical Center, Worcester, Massachusetts.
4 Prior investigators
had metal weights custom smithed at great expense in order to make them
visually identical. In the present study, plastic vials were coated with
black paint and filled with varying amounts of sand to achieve the same
result at a considerable cost savings.
5 Decreased n
at evaluation 2 because one participants questionnaire was missing. At
evaluation 1, this participant did not mark any items.
REFERENCES
Ahles,
T. A., & Blanchard, B. B., Leventhal, H. (1983). Cognitive control
of pain: attention to the sensory aspects of the cold pressor stimulus.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7 (2), 159-178.
Barr, B. P.,
& Benson, H. (1984). The relaxation response and cardiovascular disorders.
Behavioral Medicine Update, 6 (4), 28-30.
Benson,
H. (1983). The relaxation response: its subjective and objective historical
precedents and physiology. Trends in Neurosciences, 6 (7), 281-284.
Benson,
H., & Klipper, M. Z. (1988). The relaxation response (2nd ed.).
New York: Avon.
Delmonte,
M. M. (1984). Meditation Practice as related to occupational stress, health,
and productivity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59 (2), 581-582.
DeLongis,
A., Coyne, J. C., Dakof, G., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1982).
Relationship of daily hassles, uplifts, and major life events to health
status. Health Psychology, 1, 119-136.
Derogatis,
L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R administration, scoring, and procedures manual.
Towson, MD: Clinical Psychometric Research.
Dubreuil,
D., & Kohn, P. (1986). Reactivity and response to pain. Personality
and Individual Differences, 7, 907-909.
Duncan,
G. H., Bushnell, M. C., & Lavigne, G. J. (1989). Comparison of verbal
and visual analogue scales for measuring the intensity and unpleasantness
of experimental pain. Pain, 37, 295-303.
Forgione,
A. G., & Barber, T. X. (1971). A strain gauge pain stimulator. Psychophysiology,
8, 102-106.
Garfinkel,
M. S., Schumacher, H. R. Jr., Husain, A., Levy, M., & Reshetar, R.
A. (1994). Evaluation of a yoga based regimen for treatment of osteoarthritis
of the hands. Journal of Rheumatology, 21 (12), 2341-2343.
Goldstein,
J. (1987). The experience of insight: a simple and direct guide to Buddhist
meditation. Boston, MA: Shambhala Publications, Inc.
Haslam,
D. R. (1970). Lateral dominance in the perception of pain and size. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 503-507.
Kabat-Zinn,
J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain
patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation. General Hospital
Psychiatry, 4, 33-47.
Kabat-Zinn,
J., Lipworth, L., & Burney, R. (1985). The clinical use of mindfulness
meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 8, 163-190.
Kabat-Zinn,
J., Lipworth, L., Burney, R., & Sellers, W. (1986). Four year follow-up
of a meditation-based program for the self-regulation of chronic pain:
treatment outcomes and compliance. The Clinical Journal of Pain, 2,
159-173.
Kabat-Zinn,
J., Massion, A. O., Kristeller, J., Peterson, L. G., Fletcher, K. E., Pbert,
L., Lenderking, W. R., & Santorelli, S. F. (1992). Effectiveness of
a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety
disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149 (7), 936-943.
Kanner, A. D.,
Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Comparison of
two modes of stress measurement: daily hassles and uplifts versus major
life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4 (1), 1-39.
Krishnamurti,
J. (1969). Freedom from the known. New York: Harper and Row.
Kutz, I. (1985).
Meditation as an adjunct to psychotherapy: an outcome study. Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatics, 43 (4), 209-218.
Labrecque,
G, & Vanier, N. C. (1995). Biological rhythms in pain and in the effects
of opioid analgesics. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 68 (1), 129-47.
Leventhal,
H., Brown, D., Shacham, S., & Engquist, G. (1979). Effects of preparatory
information about sensations, threat of pain, and attention on cold pressor
distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (5),
688-714.
Leventhal,
H., & Everhart, D. (1980). Emotion, pain, and physical illness. In
C. E. Izard (ed.). Emotions and psychopathology. New York: Plenum
Press.
Malouff,
J. M., Schutte, N. S., & Ramerth, W. (1985). Evaluation of a short
form of the POMS-Depression Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41,
389-391.
Malow, R.
M., West, J. A., & Sutker, P. B. (1987). A sensory decision theory
analysis of anxiety and pain responses in chronic drug abusers. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 96 (3), 184-189.
Malow, R.
M., West, J. A., & Sutker, P. B. (1989). Anxiety and pain response
changes across treatment: sensory decision analysis. Pain, 38 (1),
35-44.
McCaul,
K. D., & Haugtvedt, C. (1982). Attention, distraction, and cold-pressor
pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43 (1), 154-162.
McNair,
D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual: Profile of
Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Melzack,
R., & Wall, P. D. (1982). The challenge of pain. New York: Basic
Books.
Merskey,
H. (1974). Assessment of pain. Physiotherapy, 4, 96-98.
Mills, W.
W., & Farrow, J. T. (1981). The transcendental meditation technique
and acute experimental pain. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43 (2), 157-164.
Murray,
F. S., & Safferstone, J. F. (1970). Pain threshold and tolerance of
right and left hands. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,
71, 83-86.
Nyanaponika,
T. (1962). The heart of Buddhist meditation. New York: Samuel Weiser.
Sharma,
M. P., Kumaraiah, V., Mishra, H., & Balodhi, J. P. (1990). Therapeutic
effects of vipassana meditation in tension headache. Journal of Personality
and Clinical Studies, 6 (2), 201-206.
AUTHOR NOTE
Brian T. Hafer, Department
of Psychology, Duke University.
I thank Dr. Francis J. Keefe for assistance
in designing the experiment, for reading the manuscript, and for guidance
in making revisions. I thank Dr. Katherine Wright for assistance in leading
the meditation training sessions and in analyzing the data obtained. I
also thank all those who kindly volunteered to participate in the study.
This study was conducted in partial fulfillment of the Latin honors thesis
requirements of the Department of Psychology.